The Magic of the Personal Touch: Field Experimental Evidence on Money and Appreciation as Gifts*

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/sjoe.12310
AuthorSusanne Neckermann,Christiane Bradler
Published date01 July 2019
Date01 July 2019
Scand. J. of Economics 121(3), 1189–1221, 2019
DOI: 10.1111/sjoe.12310
The Magic of the Personal Touch: Field
Experimental Evidence on Money and
Appreciation as Gifts*
Christiane Bradler
Volkswagen, DE-38440 Wolfsburg,Germany
christianebradler@yahoo.de
Susanne Neckermann
University of Chicago, Chicago IL 60615, USA
sneckermann@uchicago.edu
Abstract
In this paper, we use two field experiments in professional settings to explore the effort levels
of individuals in response to gifts. We extend the literature by looking at non-financial gifts that
signal worker appreciation and gifts that combine financial and non-financial elements with or
without a personal touch. Wefind that while money and appreciation are individually effective,
these only work welltogether when they are combined with a personal touch. This suggests that
responses to gifts are sensitive to the presentation of the gift as well as to interpersonal elements;
these are factors that have so far been largely ignored in the literature but are easy to incorporate
into existing principal–agent models.
Keywords: Field experiment; gift exchange; gratitude; personnel economics; reciprocity
JEL classification:C93; M52
I. Introduction
The giving of gifts is a widespread phenomenon, not only in social
settings, but also in the professional realm. Many firms, for instance, use
gifts as a means of reinforcing their relationships with their clients and
employees. While gifts can serve many purposes, one objective of gift
giving, which is especially common in corporate settings, is the elicitation
of reciprocity.1
*We gratefully acknowledge comments and suggestions by numerous seminar and conference
participants. Special thanks go to Michael Kosfeld, Steve Levitt, John List, and Nick Zubanov.
We thank Ann-Kathrin Koessler, Graham Tierney, MattieToma, and Jeannine van Reeken for
excellent research assistance.
1Mauss (1954) was the first to systematicallydocument the role of gifts for establishing reciprocal
relationships.
C
The editors of The Scandinavian Journal of Economics 2018.
1190 Field experimental evidence on money and appreciation as gifts
Gifts utilized by businesses can take many different forms. Some gifts
are purely financial, such as a year-end bonus, a rebate, or above market-
clearing wages. Other gifts (e.g., a thank you card or a pat on the back) have
no financial component. In many cases, gifts have both a financial and a
non-financial component (for instance, an expression of worker appreciation
accompanied by a financial gift).
When gifts include both financial and non-financial elements, these
two components can be combined in more or less thoughtful ways, where
thoughtfulness can, for example, be conveyed by adding a personal touch.
Consider a supervisor seeking to express her appreciation for her workers
by giving them a financial gift. One approach would be to send a mass
e-mail to all employees expressing her appreciation and noting that a bonus
will be included in the upcoming paycheck. Another approach would be to
write a series of personalized emails that convey this message. Alternatively,
she could take the time to stop by each employee’s desk, put a hand
on the employee’s shoulder, and deliver a heartfelt speech of appreciation
while handing the employee a check in an envelope with a hand-written
message.
Although few people would dispute that the way in which a gift
is presented affects the reaction of the receiver, the existing literature
– outlined below – has so far largely ignored non-financial gifts and
interpersonal elements (e.g., a personal touch) in gift giving. Instead,
it has mainly focused on financial gifts (cash transfers). This is not
surprising as cash is the most clear-cut and clean way of bestowing a
benefit on a recipient. The study of non-financial gifts and interpersonal
elements, by comparison, is inherently complicated because the particular
form of any non-pecuniary gift and any personal element can always
be criticized as context-specific, which might render any effect found as
less generalizable than any effect found in response to money. While we
recognize these inherent difficulties associated with moving away from
financial gifts, the prevalence and importance of non-financial gifts and
personal elements in real-world gift giving show the importance of studying
them.
We use two field experiments in professional settings to explore the
extent to which financial and non-financial gifts, and their combinations
with and without a personal touch, elicit reciprocity from the recipients.
We use cash as a financial reward and convey gratitude and appreciation
(the non-financial gift) via a thank you card.2We combine the financial and
the non-financial gift in two different ways: one that requires a significant
2While wedo not think that a tangible object is necessary to convey gratitude and appreciation, we
opted for a tangible object in order to make the non-financial gift more salient and meaningful.
This is in keeping with the literature on non-financial awards where researchers often confer
C
The editors of The Scandinavian Journal of Economics 2018.
C. Bradler and S. Neckermann 1191
investment of time and effort (the gift has a personal touch) and one that
requires little investment of time and effort (the gift does not have – or
has less of – a personal touch). In both experiment, we use a handmade
element as a personal touch.3
In the first experiment, Experiment 1, we hired more than 380 workers
for a three-hour data-entry job. Workers were paid a flat wage of 25
euros and were not aware that they were taking part in an experiment.
The task consisted of manually entering surveys into a database. After
the workers worked for roughly 100 minutes, the treatment intervention
took place. Depending on the treatment, the workers received no gift,
a monetary gift (5 euros), a thank you card, or both the monetary gift
and thank you card, either with or without a handmade element. The
handmade element consisted of money folded like a bow tie and attached
to the card. After the treatment intervention, workers worked for another
hour.
The results show that both the monetary gift as well as the thank
you card alone significantly increased the performance of the workers,
compared with workers who did not receive a gift. These effect sizes are not
significantly different from one another, and thus suggest that a thank you
card and a 5 euro gift are equally effective in this setting. Interestingly,
however, the combination of 5 euros and the thank you card without a
handmade element does not increase performance, while the combination
with a handmade element is very effective (the effect size is roughly double
the effect size of the 5 euros and thank you card alone). The results suggest
that the combination of money and gratitude only works when the giver
adds a personal touch to the gift.
To test the robustness of these results and to investigate the underlying
mechanism, we designed a second field experiment, Experiment 2, using a
different setting and a non-student subject pool. We sent out 1,570 letters
asking recipients to fill out and return a 10-minute survey. Depending on
the treatment, the letter either contained a monetary gift, a combination
of a monetary gift and a thank you card without a personal touch, or a
combination of the two with a personal touch. This replicates our three
main treatments from Experiment 1.4The handmade element consisted
of a hand-drawn sketch of a flower on the thank you card. The outcome
tokens, such as certificates or trophies, on to award winners to symbolize their achievements,
making sure that the tokens themselves have no value or meaning; for example, Kosfeld and
Neckermann (2011) used a certificate, Ashraf et al. (2014) used a congratulator y letter from the
minister of health, and Levitt et al. (2016) used a trophy.
3Please refer to Section II for a definition and discussion of what constitutes a personal touch.
4Toincrease statistical power, we limited the set of main treatments to the twocombinations with
and without a personal touch and a reference treatment, money alone (1 euro, approx. US$1.1).
C
The editors of The Scandinavian Journal of Economics 2018.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT